'Pay attention and you might miss it': Psychology researchers show we’re learning even when we’re not paying attention

February 24, 2023 by Chris Sasaki - A&S News

When Alexandra Decker was taking her multiple-choice driving exam recently, she realized that the correct answer was always the longest and most detailed one provided.

“If I had been focused on just the lesson material and choosing the right answer,” she says, “I might not have picked up on that pattern.”

Decker is a postdoctoral fellow at the McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, and a former graduate student in the labs of Amy Finn and Katherine Duncan, both associate professors in the Faculty of Arts & Science’s Department of Psychology.

What Decker experienced was our ability to learn secondary or non-targeted information — like a pattern in the answers to a test — when our attention on the primary or targeted information lapses.

She and Michael Dubois are co-lead authors of a recently published paper describing their study into that ability: Pay attention and you might miss it: Greater learning during attentional lapses. It was published in the journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. In addition to Decker and Dubois, the co-authors were Duncan and Finn.

Our conclusion is that these attentional lapses can be helpful for learning because when we're too focused or too fixated, we miss information in our environment that might be useful.

“We experience attentional lapses all the time and, generally, they're considered a bad thing,” says Decker, who worked on the study before joining MIT. “But we wanted to know whether being too focused on a learning goal might not be a good thing and whether attentional lapses can broaden our attention and enable us to take in more information from our environment.”

What the researchers found was that study participants demonstrated heightened learning of non-targeted information when their attention to targeted information lapsed. They also showed that participants who spent more time in a poor attentional state displayed the greatest learning about peripheral information.

“Our conclusion is that these attentional lapses can be helpful for learning because when we're too focused or too fixated, we miss information in our environment that might be useful,” says Decker.

An attention lapse is not necessarily a bad thing

Adds Dubois, “A lot of work has been done to try and improve attention and stay focused longer. But our thinking is that we should reconceptualize lapses in attention as opportunities to learn something else. In other words, an attention lapse is not necessarily a bad thing.”

The result provides insight into a long-standing debate about two contrasting theories of attention lapses.

According to the overload or depletion theory, we have a finite amount of attention to give and when we use it up, we learn neither goal-relevant nor goal-irrelevant information.

But the study supports underload theories which say that attentional lapses promote learning of goal-irrelevant content because lapses in attention are not due to the depletion of an attention resource. Rather, attention is diverted away from the main task — for example, when it becomes too monotonous.

The authors conducted their research by enlisting 53 undergraduate University of Toronto students. The students were instructed to observe a series of slides, each displaying a letter or number in the centre, flanked by two non-alphanumeric symbols — for example, #, * and @.

Because there's a lot of information to be learned from our environment that isn't obvious. Our study shows that perhaps we need to acknowledge that there’s more than one way of learning.

The students’ targeted task — which became monotonous because it was so simple — was to concentrate on the central symbol and press the "f” key if it was a letter, or a "j” key if it was number. They were told to ignore the flanking symbols and were not told there was a pattern, that one symbol was more likely to appear beside letters and another symbol beside numbers.

Despite these instructions and despite not knowing that different symbols were associated with numbers and letters, students did indeed notice the pattern.

The study may explain how people with reduced attentional control, including aging adults, children and young adults with high impulsivity, demonstrate better learning of goal-irrelevant information.

The researchers also speculate that we have attention lapses, not just because it's hard to maintain attention for a long time, but because it’s helpful in learning things you don't necessarily think are relevant in the moment.

“Our ability to focus and learn may come at the cost of learning about a lot of other information that we don’t think is relevant at the moment,” says Finn.

“Because there's a lot of information to be learned from our environment that isn't obvious. Our study shows that perhaps we need to acknowledge that there’s more than one way of learning.”